Page 213 - 6484
P. 213

alternative  and  rank  them  according  to  our  preferences.  Anyone  who  has  recently
               purchased a new laptop computer or cell phone can attest to the challenge of sorting
               through  the  different  strengths  and  limitations  of  each  brand,  model,  and  plans
               offered for support and arriving at the solution that best meets their needs.

                      In fact, the availability of too much information can lead to analysis paralysis,
               where more and more time is spent on gathering information and thinking about it,
               but no decisions actually get made. A senior executive at Hewlett-Packard admits that
               his company suffered from this spiral of analyzing things for too long to the point
               where  data  gathering  led  to  “not  making  decisions,  instead  of  us  making
                            [6]
               decisions.”   Moreover,  you  may  not  always  be  interested  in  reaching  an  optimal
               decision. For example, if you are looking to purchase a house, you may be willing
               and able to invest a great deal of time and energy to find your dream house, but if you
               are looking for an apartment to rent for the academic year, you may be willing to take
               the first one that meets your criteria of being clean, close to campus, and within your
               price range.
                      Making “Good Enough” Decisions
                      The bounded rationality model of decision making recognizes the limitations of
               our decision-making processes. According to this model, individuals knowingly limit
               their options to a manageable set and choose the best alternative without conducting
               an exhaustive search for alternatives. An important part  of the bounded rationality
               approach is the tendency to satisfice, which refers to accepting the first alternative
               that  meets  your  minimum  criteria.  For  example,  many  college  graduates  do  not
               conduct  a  national  or  international  search  for  potential  job  openings;  instead,  they
               focus their search on a limited geographic area and tend to accept the first offer in
               their chosen area, even if it may not be the ideal job situation. Satisficing is similar to
               rational decision making, but it differs in that rather than choosing the best choice and
               maximizing  the  potential  outcome,  the  decision  maker  saves  time  and  effort  by
               accepting the first alternative that meets the minimum threshold.
                      Making Intuitive Decisions
                      The intuitive decision-making model has  emerged  as  an  important  decision-
               making model. It refers to arriving at decisions without conscious reasoning. Eighty-
               nine percent of managers surveyed admitted to using intuition to make decisions at
                                                                                 [7]
               least sometimes, and 59% said they used intuition often.   When we recognize that
               managers often need to make decisions under challenging circumstances with time
               pressures,  constraints,  a  great  deal  of  uncertainty,  highly  visible  and  high-stakes
               outcomes, and within changing conditions, it makes sense that they would not have
               the time to formally work through all the steps of the rational decision-making model.
               Yet  when  CEOs,  financial  analysts,  and  healthcare  workers  are  asked  about  the
               critical decisions they make, seldom do they attribute success to luck. To an outside
               observer, it may seem like they are making guesses as to the course of action to take,
               but it turns out that they are systematically making decisions using a different model
               than was earlier suspected. Research on life-or-death decisions made by fire chiefs,
               pilots, and nurses finds that these experts do not choose among a list of well-thought-
               out alternatives. They don’t decide between two or three options and choose the best
               one. Instead, they consider only one option at a time. The intuitive decision-making


                                                              213
   208   209   210   211   212   213   214   215   216   217   218