Page 34 - 6688
P. 34

34
                    Goffman's SCs are claimed to operate in all communication, including written text. There
              are many  ways that  authors  give readers  turns. Textbook writers, for  example,  have subtle
              ways of taking the reader's contribution into account – assuming that the reader would have
              objected to X, or assuming that the reader has a question Y and the textbook writer answers it.
              Summaries at the end of units serve the function of answering the reader's unspoken question,
              "What is the most important information, and why is it important?" Conversely, in composition
              classes, teachers instruct novice writers to consider their readers as if they were about to ask
              not just for summaries but also for definitions, for examples, or other evidence, and even as if
              they  were  about  to  argue  with  the  writer.  That  is,  written  communication  is  a  reciprocal
              transaction between the writer and the reader.

                    4.Acoustically adequate and interpretable messages
                    Communication  requires  an  ungarbled  message.  In  order  for  communication  to  take
              place, messages have to be interpretable. They also have to be "bearable." We have all been
              at cocktail parties or receptions in crowded rooms where the noise level prohibits message
              reception. If messages are garbled, they must be "repaired." If they are not, then other parts of
              the communication system break down, and communication grinds to a halt.
                    The question is What constitutes a clear message? Just how acoustically accurate must
              a message be to be "adequate," and what makes a message "interpretable"? How clear must
              messages be in order to serve communication?
                    Sometimes language learners may make many phonological and syntactic errors, and
              yet communication takes place.
                    Gumperz (1979: 15) says that "participants need not agree on the details of what was
              meant in any utterance, so long as they have negotiated a common theme or focus."
                    When people are learning languages, they may have difficulty interpreting messages not
              negotiated to their level of competence. There are many ways to deal with this. Some learners
              "fake it," pretending to understand and continuing to interact in the hope that they will catch the
              theme or focus of the conversation. Consequently, native speakers may judge the learner’s
              competence as quite high, when in reality he/she does not understand much of what is said.
                    Obviously, there are pluses and minuses to the use of faking strategies. Communication
              can continue fairly smoothly, but it may  also break  down completely since information that
              allows the participants to build a common theme or focus is missing.
                     Other learners use backchannel cues to let the speaker know they do not understand.
               The speaker then repairs the message. As the talk is negotiated, repairs and readjustments
               are made, and the talk becomes simplified to an appropriate level. There are advantages and
               disadvantages to this strategy too. The message becomes comprehensible during the repair
               process, but both the native speaker and language learner may find the need for constant
               negotiation of repairs too burdensome to make the conversation worthwhile. The learner may
               then be denied the extended interaction with native speakers that could facilitate language
               learning.
                    There is a whole body of research that looks at the types of adjustments we make when
              our messages are not acoustically adequate or interpretable. In conversational analysis, this
              includes  the  study  of  the  "repair"  system  (Sacks,  Schegloff,  and  Jefferson  1974;  Schegloff
              1979). In research on language learning, the repairs often lead to special registers such as
              "foreigner talk," "teacher talk," or (in the case of language disorder research) "clinician talk."
                    These registers have many similarities. All show an increase in acoustical accuracy. This
              is accomplished by slowing the rate of speech so that there are fewer reduced vowels, fewer
   29   30   31   32   33   34   35   36   37   38   39